Showing 51 - 60 of 528
Gill and Prowse (2023) study response times using a repeated p-beauty contest (p = 0.7). Looking at between-subject variation in response times, they found that subjects who think for longer, on average, win more rounds and choose lower numbers. When comparing average response times and level-k...
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10015074003
Response times are a simple low-cost indicator of the process of reasoning in strategic games (Rubinstein, 2007; Rubinstein, 2016). We leverage the dynamic nature of response-time data from repeated strategic interactions to measure the strategic complexity of a situation by how long people think on...
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10011653246
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10010473559
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10011754635
Response times are a simple low-cost indicator of the process of reasoning in strategic games (Rubinstein, 2007; Rubinstein, 2016). We leverage the dynamic nature of response-time data from repeated strategic interactions to measure the strategic complexity of a situation by how long people think on...
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10011607565
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10012163499
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10011942721
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10012223835
Gill and Prowse (2023) study response times using a repeated p-beauty contest (p = 0.7). Looking at between-subject variation in response times, they found that subjects who think for longer, on average, win more rounds and choose lower numbers. When comparing average response times and level-k...
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10015078206
Persistent link: https://www.econbiz.de/10014434200